Perkins School for the Blind Transition Center

The Critical Need for Tracing Individual Trajectories After an Early Diagnosis of Autism

Autism Spectrum Disorder (here forward referred to autism) is more commonly recognized at early ages (Zwaigenbaum, Bauman, Choueiri, et al., 2015). This has led to increased access to early intervention and special educational supports, which in turn provide opportunities to better long-term outcomes (Zwaigenbaum, Bauman, Choueiri, et al., 2015; Dawson et al., 2010; Lovas, 1987). Given the early onset and long-term nature of autism, its presentation changes over time and throughout the course of life. Thus, tracking each individual’s development over time is as critical as early diagnosis. Tracking over time includes not only frequent objective monitoring within the area targeted by a specific current intervention – as required in evidence-based approaches – but also regular comprehensive follow-up using neurodevelopmental and psychological assessments. Building from initial diagnostic observations, these follow-up assessments allow for development of longitudinal trajectories that, in turn, can inform intervention programs. Although to date, there is not a single schedule for follow-up assessments that fits all, here, we discuss the evidence supporting the need for regular follow-up in the preschool years, following an early diagnosis.

Adriana Di Martino, MD

Adriana Di Martino, MD

Bethany Vibert, PsyD

Bethany Vibert, PsyD

Cynthia Martin, PsyD

Cynthia Martin, PsyD

Early Childhood: A Time of Change and Opportunities

The first five years of life are a time of significant change in all children and are a critical and sensitive window for opportunities in autism. This developmental period is characterized by dynamic progress in multiple domains of functioning ranging from motor and sensory processing to language and social cognition. In this context, particularly prior to age 36 months, core symptoms of autism begin to unfold (Kim et al., 2018; Wetherby et al., 2004; Stone et al., 2000; Coonrod & Stone, 2004). Several studies following the same children throughout early childhood (e.g. Kim et al, 2018) and into adulthood (e.g., Lord et al, 2012), have revealed that the trajectory of core autism symptoms varies tremendously across children, ranging from minimal to large improvements and vary in rate of change. Studies of younger siblings at risk for autism have shown that while many toddlers meet full diagnostic criteria for autism at 18-24 months, others will not have a clear presentation until 30-36 months (Messinger et al., 2013; Chawarska et al., 2014; Ozonoff et al., 2015), and some will be recognized even later (Miodovnik et al., 2015).

Cognition, Language and Autistic Symptoms are Intertwined

Variable trajectories of autism symptoms are not independent from co-occurring changes in cognitive and language skills. Verbal and nonverbal reasoning paint the background against which all other skills develop. They, in turn, can impact the expression of core autism symptoms. The development of cognitive and language skills is paramount in the early years and have been shown to lead to better long-term prognosis (e.g., Simonoff et al., 2019), even with the persistence of autism symptoms. Thus, they need to be carefully monitored to identify specific areas of strengths and weaknesses that can serve either as supports or targets for interventions. Profound cognitive delays that extend past early childhood can require long-term comprehensive support plans.

Language skills are another important contributor to positive outcomes. Verbal skills change significantly over time and their trajectories vary across individuals ranging from reaching age-appropriate verbal fluency to remaining minimally verbal (Anderson et al, 2007). Nonverbal aspects of social communication and interaction are also important areas to monitor for intervention. For example, non-verbal communication skills, such as joint attention and spontaneous use of gestures are critical in further promoting language development (Murray et al, 2008; Bono, Daley, & Sigman, 2004; Mundy, Sigman, Kasari, 1990).

For children who develop verbal fluency, the expression of autism symptoms may change within the context of language development. For example, toddlers and preschoolers who transition from being non-verbal to phrase- or fluent- speech, may start showing atypical use of language, which can be transitory or persist. This may include frequent use of repetitive phrases, idiosyncratic speech, and scripting lines from books or movies. Follow-up assessments will determine the extent to which these atypical language patterns are either interfering with social communication or used to aid social interactions and inform treatment accordingly.

The Relevance of Behavioral Regulation

Behavioral regulation (i.e, the ability to control motor behavior, sustain attention and regulate emotions appropriately to a given context) broadly affects a child’s functioning. Poor behavior regulation can challenge social functioning, may later affect academic achievement, or require placement in restrictive academic settings – regardless of intellectual and language capabilities (Sansosti & Sansosti, 2012). When autism symptoms co-occur with dysregulated behavior, it is important to identify the triggers early so they can be promptly targeted in behavioral interventions. Improvements (decreases) in hyperactive behaviors around 24 months of age to age 36 months, for example, have been reported to characterize a group of individuals who, by young adulthood, have a very positive outcome with minimal-to-no symptoms of autism and greater adaptive functioning (Anderson et al, 2014). In this group, in addition to decreases in hyperactivity, the most significant early childhood characteristic was the magnitude of reductions in the severity of restricted repetitive behaviors (RRB) between 24- and 36-months age (Anderson et al, 2014). RRB is an umbrella term that captures both lower- and higher-order symptoms. The former includes symptoms such atypical sensory processing and motor stereotypes, the latter includes symptoms such as excessive adherence to routines and restricted interest. The relative role of behavior regulation and RRB, particularly their interaction, on long-term outcomes remains to be fully understood, but is an important area of clinical focus in follow-up assessments and interventions for autism.

Guidelines for Assessment Battery

When reassessing developmental functioning and core autism symptoms, standardized testing is required. Some testing instruments provide greater utility in monitoring trajectory than others. Integrating gold-standard testing measures (i.e., measures that have been developed and validated based on robust and rigorous research), represent a common framework across providers and institutions, which can facilitate coordinated care. Standardized tests such as the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995) and Differential Ability Scales, Second Edition (DAS-2; Elliot et al., 2007) quantifies a child’s developmental functioning across verbal and nonverbal cognitive domains and have been used extensively in the research literature. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al, 2012) to date is the gold-standard diagnostic instrument for autism. It is often used to track major changes over time. Standardized instruments that are more sensitive to subtle changes, specifically the Brief Observation of Social Communication Change (BOSCC), have been developed (Grzadzinski et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2019) and independent validation studies are underway (Kitzerow, et al., 2016). Moreover, assessments provided by clinician(s) not directly involved in the child’s daily educational and treatment plan, may bypass potential biases and provide objective markers of change over time. Repeating follow-up neurodevelopmental and psychological assessments at least yearly, or more frequently if new concerns emerge, is recommended. Families can access these assessments through most autism specialty clinics and research programs.

As part of a study funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; R01MH115363-03), the Child Mind Institute is currently offering autism diagnostic and yearly follow-up evaluations of behavioral and cognitive changes in toddlers and preschoolers with autism who are interested and eligible in participating in research.

Cynthia Martin, PsyD, and Bethany A. Vibert, PsyD, are clinical psychologists and Adriana Di Martino, MD, is Research Director at the Autism Center at the Child Mind Institute.

To learn more about the Autism Center at the Child Mind Institute, visit www.childmind.org/center/autism-center.

References

Anderson DK, Liang JW, Lord C. Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(5):485-494. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12178

Bono, MA., Daley, T., Sigman, M. (2004). Relations Among Joint Attention, Amount of Intervention and Language Gain in Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 495-505.

Chawarska, K., Shic, F., Macari, S., Campbell, D. J., Brian, J., Landa, R., … & Young, G. S. (2014). 18-month predictors of later outcomes in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(12), 1317-1327.

Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., … & Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125(1), e17-e23.

Coonrod, E., and Stone, W. (2004). Early Concerns of Parents of Children With Autistic and Nonautistic Disorders. Infants & Young Children, 17(3), 258-268

Elliott, C. D., Salerno, J. D., Dumont, R., & Willis, J. O. (2007). Differential ability scales, Second edition. San Antonio, TX.

Grzadzinski, R., Carr, T., Colombi, C. et al. Measuring Changes in Social Communication Behaviors: Preliminary Development of the Brief Observation of Social Communication Change (BOSCC). J Autism Dev Disord 46, 2464–2479 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2782-9

Kim, S. H., Bal, V. H., Benrey, N., Choi, Y. B., Guthrie, W., Colombi, C., & Lord, C. (2018). Variability in Autism Symptom Trajectories Using Repeated Observations From 14 to 36 Months of Age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(11), 837–848.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.05.026

Kim, S. H., Grzadzinski, R., Martinez, K., & Lord, C. (2019). Measuring treatment response in children with autism spectrum disorder: Applications of the Brief Observation of Social Communication Change to the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Autism, 23(5), 1176–1185. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318793253

Lord, C., Luyster, R., Guthrie, W., & Pickles, A. (2012). Patterns of developmental trajectories in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 80(3), 477–489. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027214

Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (pp. 58-64). Circle Pines, MN: AGS.

Lovaas O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.55.1.3

Kitzerow, J., Teufel, K., Wilker, C. and Freitag, C.M. (2016), Using the brief observation of social communication change (BOSCC) to measure autism‐specific development. Autism Research, 9: 940-950. doi:10.1002/aur.1588

Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., Landa, R. J., Charman, T., Stone, W. L., Constantino, J. N., Hutman, T., Carver, L. J., Bryson, S., Iverson, J. M., Strauss, M. S., Rogers, S. J., & Sigman, M. (2013). Beyond autism: a baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011

Miodovnik, A., Harstad, E., Sideridis, G., & Huntington, N. (2015). Timing of the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Autism Spectrum Disorder. Pediatrics 136 (4) e830-e837; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-1502

Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115-128.

Murray, D. S., Creaghead, N. A., Manning-Courtney, P., Shear, P. K., Bean, J., & Prendeville, J.-A. (2008). The Relationship Between Joint Attention and Language in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 23(1), 5–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357607311443

Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Landa, R. J., Brian, J., Bryson, S., Charman, T., … & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2015). Diagnostic stability in young children at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(9), 988-998.

Sansosti, J. M., & Sansosti, F. J. (2012). Inclusion for students with high‐functioning autism spectrum disorders: Definitions and decision making. Psychology in the Schools, 49(10), 917-931.

Simonoff, E., Kent, R., Stringer, D., Lord, C., Briskman, J., Lukito, S., … & Baird, G. (2019). Trajectories in symptoms of autism and cognitive ability in autism from childhood to adult life: Findings from a longitudinal epidemiological cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.

Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., & Ousley, O. Y. (2000). Brief report: Screening tool for autism in two-year-olds (STAT): Development and preliminary data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(6), 607-12. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1005647629002

Zwaigenbaum, L., Bauman, M., Choueiri, R., Kasari, C., Carter, A., et al. (2015). Early Intervention for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Under 3 Years of Age: Recommendations for Practice and Research. Pediatrics, 136 (Supplement 1) S60-S81; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-3667E

Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., et al (2004). Early Indicators of Autism Spectrum Disorders in the Second Year of Life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 34(5), 473-493.

Have a Comment?